Newses & Articles

Does Food Color Influence Taste and Flavor Perceptionin Humans?Charles Spence & Carmel A. Levitan &Maya U. Shankar & Massimiliano Zampini
Publish Date : 1395/6/6 Time 12:41:03
distinguish between the evidence pertinent to evaluating
two relatively independent research questions: (1) Does the
presence versus absence, or change in the intensity, of
the color present in a food or drink influence peoples
perception of the intensity of a particular flavor (e.g.,
banana, strawberry, etc.) or taste (such as sweetness,
sourness, etc.)? (2) Does food coloring influence the correct
identification of a food or drinks flavor?
The evidence concerning the first question is indeed
rather mixed (Auvray and Spence 2008; Stevenson 2009;
Zampini et al. 2007), with some researchers observing a
significant effect of changing the intensity of the coloring
added to a food on peoples judgments of flavor and/or taste
intensity (e.g., Johnson and Clydesdale 1982; Johnson et al.
1982, 1983;Rothetal.1988), while many others have failed
to demonstrate any such effect (e.g., Alley and Alley 1998;
Frank et al. 1989;seeTable1). By contrast, the published
evidence unequivocally supports an affirmative answer to the
second question, with peoples judgments of a flavors
identity often being reliably affected by a foodscolor,beit
appropriate, inappropriate, or absent. Note here that we are
not aware of any research having been conducted assessing
the effect of color on the identification of basic tastes (e.g.,
when solutions containing an odorless tastant such as sugar
or salt are sampled). Below, we review the evidence relating
to these two questions. We discuss several of the possible
mechanisms that may underlie these crossmodal effects on
taste and flavor identification, such as cognitive expectancy,
bottom-up multisensory integration, and attention.
Which Senses Contribute to the Perception of Flavor?
Before we proceed, however, it is important to note that
gustatory, olfactory, and oralsomatosensory cues (in
contrast to visual and auditory cues) all contribute directly
to flavor perception. In fact, the International Standards
Organization (ISO 5492 1992) has defined flavor as a
complex combination of the olfa ctory, gustatory and
trigeminal sensations perceived during tasting. The flavor
may be influenced by tactile, thermal, painful and/or
kinaesthetic effects (see Delwiche 2004, p. 137; see also
ISO 5492 2008). Visual and auditory cues may modify a
foods flavor, but they are not, at least according to the ISO
definition, intrinsic to it (though see also Auvray and
Spence 2008; Stevens on 2009 for alternative views). Visual
cues, such as a f oods color, may then modify the
perception of a foods flavor by influencing the gustatory
qualities of the food, by influencing the olfac tory attributes
of the food (as perceived orthonasally and/or retronasally;
Koza et al. 2005), by influencing the oralsomatosensory
qualities of the food, and/or by influencing the overall
multisensory flavor percept (or Gestalt; see Fig. 1). In this
article, we review the evidence regarding colors effect on
taste and flavor. We also discuss the evidence regarding
colors influence on olfactory judgments where relevant.
1
Does Food Color Influe nce Perceived Taste or Flavor
Intensity?
The evidence pertaining to the question of whether food
coloring influences peoples perception (or ratings) of taste
or flavor intensity is currently rathe r ambiguous: that is,
while a number of studies have demonstrated a significant
effect of increasing the level of food coloring on peoples
ratings of taste or flavor intensity across a range of different
drinks (Hyman 1983; Johnson and Clydesdale 1982;
Johnson et al. 1982, 1983; Kostyla 1978; Romeu and De
Vicente 1968; Roth e t al. 1988), many other studies have
either failed to demonstrate any such crossmodal effect (e.g.,
Alley and Alley 1998; Chan and Kane-Martinelli 1997;
Frank et al. 1989; Gifford and Clydesdale 1986; Gifford et
al. 1987), or else have demonstrated complex (and/or
unexpected) interactions that have proved rather more
difficult to interpret (e.g., Christensen 1985; DuBose et al.
1980, experiment 1; Fletcher et al. 1991; Lavin and
Lawless 1998; McCullough et al. 1978 ; Pangborn 1960;
Strugnell 1997; Zampini et al. 2007; experiment 2).
2
Given
that the evidence regarding colors influence on taste
intensity would appear to be rather more ambiguous than
its effect on flavor intensity, the evidence pertaining to each
of these is dealt with separately in the following sections.
Does food color influence taste intensity? One of the
classic studies to investigate colors influence on taste
sensitivity was conducted by Maga (1974). He investigated
the effects of coloring an aqueous solution red, green, or
1
It should be noted that the near absence of research means that, as
yet, there is nothing much to say about visions influence, if any, on
the oralsomatosensory attributes of flavor (see Christensen 1983;de
Wijk et al. 2004; Frost and Janhoj 2007 for exceptions; see Verhagen
and Engelen 2006 for a review).
2
Over the years, researchers have looked at colors role in influencing
peoples perception of the taste and flavor of many different foods,
including jellies (Moir 1936), cake (DuBose et al. 1980; Moir 1936;
Tom et al. 1987), chocolate (Duncker 1939; Levitan et al. 2008;
Shankar et al. 2009), syrups (Kanig 1955), sherbets (Hall 1958), wine
gums (Tee rling 1992), and yogurt (Norton and Johnson 1987).
However, more recently, the majority of the research has tended to
use various different drinks, beverages, and solutions as the stimuli of
choice. Most likely, this research focus on colored drinks (be they
carbonated or uncarbonated) reflects both the ease of stimulus control
and creation that such experimental materials afford and also the fact
that color provides one of the few distinctive non-olfactory features of
such stimuli (see Christensen 1985; Oram et al. 1995). Given this bias
in the literature, we have also chosen to focus our review primarily on
those studies that have investigated the effect of color on taste and
flavor perception in various solutions, drinks, and beverages.
Chem. Percept. (2010) 3:6884 69
yellow on perceptual thresholds for four of the basic tastes
(salty, sour, sweet, and bitter). Note that each of the basic
tastes was tested in a separate part of the experiment,
meaning that the participants were presumably never
uncertain with regard to the identity of the tastant whose
presence they were trying to detect. In many cases, Maga
observed that the concentration of the tastant had to be
increased in order for his participants to be able to detect its
presence in the colored (as compared to the uncolored)
solutions. So, for example, the addition of green coloring
to a sweet solution significantly increased taste sensitivity,
while yellow color decreased taste sensitivity (see Table 2).
Interestingly, red coloring had no significant effect on
sensitivity to sweet taste. With respect to sour taste
sensitivity, both the yellow and green coloring of solutions
decreased participants sensitivity, with red coloring again
having no effect. Coloring a clear solution red decreased
bitter taste sensitivity, while the addition of yellow and green
coloring had no such effect. Finally, adding color had no
effect on taste detection thresholds for salt solutions.
Johnson and Clydesdale (1982), in an oft-cited study,
demonstrated an effect of food coloring on taste perception
in sweetened solutions (that sometimes contai ned a cherry
flavoring). The participants in their study had to perform
both a threshold task and a magnitude estimation task. In
the threshold task, Johnson and Clydesdale found that on
average, when odorless solutions were colored red, partic-
ipants could more easily detect the presence of sucrose than
when they were uncolo red (cf. Maga 1974), though the
intensity of the color did not have a significant effect on
their performance. In the magnitude estimation task,
however, Johnson and Clydesdale found that changing the
level of food coloring had a significant effect on partic-
ipants perception of the sweetness of both odorless and
cherry-flavored solutions, with the darker-colored solutions
being rated as 210% sweeter than the lighter-colored
Table 1 Summary of the studies that have been published to date that have investigated the effect of varying the presence vs. absence, the
appropriateness/inappropriateness, or the intensity, of the color added to a solution on participants taste and/or flavor (Fl) intensity ratings. The table
highlights the fact that the majority of research in this area has focused on the influence of color on sweetness perception. The table also highlights the
inconsistency in the pattern of results that has been reported to date across the various studies that have been reported
Study Tastant Flavor Result
Pangborn (1960) Sw, So Sig
Pangborn et al. (1963) Sw Sig
Romeu and De Vicente (1968) Fl Sig
Maga (1974) Sw, Sa, So, Bi Sig
Kostyla (1978 ) Sw, So Fl Sig
McCullough et al. (1978) Sw Complex
DuBose et al. (1980) Fl Complex
Johnson and Clydesdale (1982) Sw Sig
Johnson et al. (1982) Sw Sig
Hyman (1983) Fl Sig
Johnson et al. (1983) Sw Sig
Gifford and Clydesdale (1986) Sa n.s.
Gifford et al. (1987) Sa n.s.
Roth et al. (1988) Sw Sig
Frank et al. (1989) Sw n.s.
Fletcher et al. (1991) Sw Complex
Philipsen et al. (1995) Sw Sig
Chan and Kane-Martinelli (1997) Sa n.s.
Strugnell (1997) Sw Sig
Alley and Alley (1998) Sw n.s.
Lavin and Lawless (1998) Sw Complex
Bayarri et al. (2001) Sw Fl Complex
Zampini et al. (2007) Fl Complex
Zampini et al. (2008) Sw, So Fl Complex
Sw sweet, Sa salt, So sour, Sig significant result, n.s. non-significant result, Complex typically a mixture of significant and non-significant results
Table 1 Summary of the studies that have been published to date that
have investigated the effect of varying the presence vs. absence, the
appropriateness/inappropriateness, or the intensity, of the color added to
a solution on participants taste and/or flavor (Fl) intensity ratings. The
table highlights the fact that the majority of research in this area has
focused on the influence of color on sweetness perception. The table
also highlights the inconsistency in the pattern of results that has been
reported to date across the various studies that have been reported
70 Chem. Percept. (2010) 3:6884
reference solutions despite the fact that the actual concen-
tration of sucrose was 1% lower. In further studies, Johnson
and his colleagues went on to use the same magnitude
estimation procedure in order to sho w that sweetness
ratings for both cherry- (Johnson et al. 1982)and
strawberry-flavored drinks (Johnson et al. 1983) increased
by 213% when the intensity of the cherry red coloring was
increased. Over the years, several further studies have also
documented a significant effect of the addition of (specifical-
ly) red food coloring on the perception of sweetness (e.g., see
Kostyla 1978; Pangborn 1960; Strugnell 1997).
Color cues have also been reported to influence parti-
cipants ratings of sourness/tartness (e.g., Kostyla 1978;
Pangborn 1960). By contrast, the null result of color on the
detection threshold for the presence of salt in aqueous solu-
tions reported by Maga (1974) has been extended in several
other well-controlled laboratory studies in which coloring
was added to more ecologically valid food substrates, such as
chicken broth (see Chan and Kane-Martinelli 1997;Gifford
and Clydesdale 1986; Gifford et al. 1987). In order to try and
explain this seemingly inconsistent effect of the addition of
color on salt and sweetness perception, Maga suggested that
color might not have any effect on salt perception because
salty foods can come in any color, and hence, people have few
direct color associations with saltiness.
Maga (1974) argued that there is a natural correlation
between redness and sweetness levels as many fruits ripen.
That is, many fruits show a transition from colors at the
green end of the spectrum, through yellow, to colors at the
red end of the spectrum (see also Brice 1954; Kostyla 1978;
Lavin and Lawless 1998; Zampini et al. 2007 on this point).
Maga suggested that prior exposure to this natural colortaste
correlation (i.e., between increases in redness and sweetness in
foods) might help explain why red/green coloring should have
such a profound effect on sweetness/sourness perception.
Consistent with this suggestion, Pangborn (1960)demon-
strated that the addition of green food coloring reduced
sweetness ratings while enhancing sourness ratings in pear
nectar, though it should be noted that Pangborn and Hansen
(1963) subsequently failed to replicate this finding. A
consistent pattern of results has been reported more recently
in a study by Lavin and Lawless (1998). The participants in
this study were given four strawberry-flavored drinks of
different colors (light and dark red and light and dark green)
to rate for sweetness using a nine-point categorical scale. The
participants rated the dark red solutions as sweeter than light
red solutions, but light green solutions as sweeter than dark
green solutions.
While the results discussed thus far in this section would
appear to demonstrate that the addition (vs. absence) of
food coloring leads to a significant effect on ratings of
the presence (vs. absence; Johnson and Clydesdale 1982;
Maga 1974) of sweetness and that increasing the intensity
Table 2 Summary of the re sults f rom Maga s(1974) study
highlighting the effect of the addition of color on participants
sensitivity to each of the four traditional basic tastants when dissolved
in solution (and compared to performance when the tastants were
presented in uncolored solutions)
Color of
solution
Taste
Sour
(citric acid)
Sweet
(sucrose)
Salty (sodium
chloride)
Bitter
(caffeine)
Red No effect No effect No effect Decrease
Yellow Decrease Decrease No effect No effect
Green Decrease Increase No effect No effect
Vision
Flavour
Oral-
Somatosensation
Fig. 1 This figure highlights the multiple ways in which visual cues
might influence flavor perception. Visual cues (such as the color of a
beverage) may exert a crossmodal influence on olfaction, gustation,
and/or on oral somatosensation. Such crossmodal effects, should they
exist, might then have a carryover effect on the experienced flavor
percept once the various unisensory cues have been integra ted.
Alternatively, however, visual information might influence flavor
perception only once the olfactory, gustatory, and/or oralsomatosen-
sory cues have been integrated into a multisensory flavor percept (so,
for example, if you bite into a filled chocolate, then look to see what
color the filling of the chocolate has, then color may well influence the
integrated flavor percept). Complicating matters still further, the
nature of the crossmodal influence of visual cues on flavor perception
may well vary with the task at hand. That is, different results may be
observed as a function of whether participants are asked to report on
the pleasantness or intensity of a flavor versus having to identify or
discriminate the flavor. There is robust psychophysical evidence that
visual (color) cues can modulate peoples perception of the identity
and intensity of both orthonasally and retronasally presented odors
(e.g., Blackwell 1995; Davis 1981; Engen 1972; Morrot et al. 2001;
Zellner et al. 1991; Zellner and Kautz 1990; Zellner and Whitten
1999). There is also convincing psychophysical evidence that visual
(color) cues can influence peoples perception of the intensity of the
basic tastants when presented in odorless solutions (Johnson and
Clydesdale 1982; Maga 1974). Interestingly, however, we are aware of
no research that has directly addressed the question of whether color
cues can influence a participants ability to discriminate (and/or
identify) the identity of basic tastants when presented in solution. To
date, there is only very limited research detailing any visual
contributions to the oralsomatosensory attributes of food. Evidence
demonstrating the robust influence of visual cues on flavor identifi-
cation is discussed in the text, as is the rather more mixed evidence
concerning the influence of visual (color) cues on perceived taste and
flavor intensity
Chem. Percept. (2010) 3:6884 71
of color can increase perceived sweetness intensity ratings
(Johnson and Clydesdale 1982 ; Johnson et al. 1982, 1983;
Roth et al. 1988), it is important to note that many other
studies have failed to demonstrate any such effect (see
Alley and Alley 1998; Christensen 1985; Frank et al. 1989;
McCullough et al. 1978;Pangborn1960; see also Johnson
and Cly desdale 1982). For example, Alley and Alley
reported no effect of the addition of color (red, blue, yellow,
and green) when compared to a clear, no-color-added
baseline on participants ratings of the sweetness of either
sweetened water or gelatine samples. Similarly, Frank et al.
(1989) reported that adding red food coloring to either an
odorless or strawberry odor-sweetened aqueous samples
failed to increase perceived sweetness ratings of the orange
red-looking drinks relative to participants assessment of the
clear drinks. Note that the participants in this latter study had
to rate the intensity of 16 stimuli on a 21-point sweetness
scale: the stimuli were generated by crossing the factors of
sweetness intensity (four levels), color (present vs. absent),
and odorant (present vs. absent). In other words, half of the
drinks in this study were uncolored. This raises the possibility
that the inclusion of so many sweetened but uncolored
solutions might have helped participants to realize that the
color was not necessarily predictive of the presence/intensity
of the taste. A similar explanation could, of course, also be
put forward to account for the null results reported by Alley
and Alley (1998). Zampini et al. (2007)alsofailedto
demonstrate an easily interpretable effect of variations in
color intensity on perceived sweetness intensity using a
labeled magnitude scale rating procedure.
Thus, with regard to the question of whether or not adding
color to an otherwise colorless substrate influences the
perceived intensity of differen t basic tastes, the answer appears
to depend on a number of factors including the particular taste
under investigation (Maga 1974) and perhaps the precise
presentation protocol adopted (such as how many incongru-
ently colored trials the participant may be presented with; see
footnote 4). Certainly , magnitude estimation typically gives
rise to more pronounced effects than other methods (though
see Giffo rd and Clydesdale 1986 for one null effect of color on
salt perception reported using the magnitude estimation
technique). The consensus view would currently seem to be
that color cues do not influence the perception of saltiness (see
Chan and Kane-Martinelli 1997; Gifford and Clydesdale 1986;
Gifford et al. 1987;Maga1974). By contrast, the perception
of sweetness can be modified by the addition of red (or green)
food coloring (e.g., see Johnson et al. 1982, 1983; Kostyla
1978; Lavin and Lawless 1998; Pangborn 1960), but
importantly, this effect is not always observed (Alley and
Alley 1998; Frank et al. 1989; Johnson
1982;McCulloughet
al. 1978; Pangborn 1960). Null effects are often reported in
studies in which colored drinks are compared to clear drinks
(though see Zampini et al. 2007 for an exception).
Taken at face value, these results would appear to
suggest that color does indeed influence peoples percep-
tion of taste intensity. However, under closer examination,
many of the findings reported above are potentially
problematic. For example, in their description of the
threshold task, Johnson and Clydesdale (1982) make no
reference to interleaving any catch trials in which
unsweetened solutions were mixed in with the sweetened
solutions. This is unfortunate because of the susceptibility
to response bias; that is, it could be argued that the presence
of the coloring in certain solutions simply l ed the
participants in those studies to expect a taste to be present,
and thus, they were more willing to respond that they did
indeed perceive a taste (cf. Engen 1972; Odgaard et al.
2003). It is important to note that the magnitude estimation
procedure is also vulnerable to demand characteristics; that
is, participants could easily be influenced by the coloring
such that their responses, rather than their actual taste
percepts, changed in response to changes in the color
intensity of the solutions (indeed, it seems possible that
under certain conditions, participants may simply want to
please the experimenter). Using signal detection techniques
(e.g., see Green and Swets 1966) could help separate out
perceptual from decisional effects here.
3
Due to space
constraints, detailing each of the various factors that might
help account for the occurrence of false positive results
(if that is what Johnson and colleagues results actually
reflect) lies beyond the scope of the present paper; for
instance, the specific tasks and ranges of stimuli used in
each study, as well as the demand characteristics of each
experiment and differences in the pools of participant
tested, might all be expected to influence participants
expectations (see Shankar et al. 2009), which could, in turn,
have influenced the pattern of results that were observed.
Does food color influence flavor intensity? As visual
inspection of Table 1 makes clear, there have been far fewer
studies of colors influence on perceived flavor intensity.
However, in contrast to the rather mixed picture regarding
the effect of color on taste intensity, the story concerning
colors effect on flavor intensity seems much more clear-cut
and convincing. In one of the most frequently cited studies
in this area, DuBose et al. (1980) reported that overall flavor
intensity was affected by color intensity, with higher color
intensity solutions giving rise to stronger flavor evaluation
3
Perceptual effects are usually defined in terms of a change in
perceptual sensitivity, such as a change in the d measure that can be
derived using signal detection theory (e.g., Green and Swets 1966).
By contrast, changes in decisional criteria, possibly reflecting a
response bias or change in the criteria for responding adopted by
participants (such as, for example, participants simply being more
likely to respond that a flavor, odor, or taste is present whenever a
color is added to a liquid; see Engen 1972), may show up in ones
measure of bias (c or beta).
72 Chem. Percept. (2010) 3:6884
responses by participants for orange- (but not for the cherry-)
flavored beverages. Meanwhile, Kostyla (1978)reportedthat
the addition of yellow color to sweetened cherry-, raspberry-,
and strawberry-flavored beverages decreased flavor ratings
by around 4%. Blue color reduced fruit flavor by 20% (and
the addition of red coloring increased sweetness by 510%).
Elsewhere, significant effects of color on flavor intensity have
also been reported by Romeu and De Vicente (1968). More
complex results have been reported in other studies where
certain colorflavor combinations appear to give rise to bigger
effectsthanothers(Bayarrietal.2001), and null results of
varying the intensity of the color on flavor intensity are rarer
(Zampini et al. 2007, 2008).
It should be noted here that people are very poor at
identifying orthonasally presented odors in the absence of any
other sensory cues regarding the odors identity (e.g., Cain
1979; Desor and Beauchamp 1974;Engen1972; Eskenazi et
al. 1986; Jönsson et al. 2005; Zellner et al. 1991). By contrast,
people appear to be much better at identifying the presence of
the basic tastes (e.g., sweet, sour, salty, and bitter; see
Bartoshuk 1975; Laing et al. 2002). A priori, then, it would
seem likely that participants judgments of odor (and hence of
flavor) identity would be much more likely to be influenced
by the presentation of an incongruent visual color cue than
would their judgments of the identity of a specific tastant.
Indeed, robust effects of color cues on judgments of odor
intensity have now been reported in numerous studies (e.g.,
Blackwell 1995; Davis 1981; Engen 1972; Morrot et al.
2001; Parr et al. 2003; Zellner and Kautz 1990; Zellner and
Whitten 1999). For example, Zellner and Whitten reported
that the addition of color (presented at one of four different
color intensity levels) influenced participa nts odor intensi-
ty responses when orthonasally sniffing red strawberry and
green mint solutions. Interestingly, ratings of the intensity
of the strawberry odor peaked at the middle color intensity,
while for the mint odor, there was a monotonic increase
with increasing color intensity. In a subsequent experiment,
Zellner and Whitten show ed that while the intensity of the
color played a large role in modulating perceived odor
intensity, the specific color that was added (be it appropriate
or inappropriate) had little effect. Given results such as these,
it would seem likely that color should also influence flavor
intensity judgments. That said, however, most of these
crossmodal studies involved investigating the effect of color
on participants orthonasal odor judgments. In this regard,
the results of a study by Koza et al. (2005) are critical. For
example, they demonstrated that while the addition of color
resulted in an increase in the perceived intensity of
orthonasally presented odors, it actually led to a reduction
in the perceived intensity of the very same odors when
presented retronasally (i.e., as they would be if participants
were making flavor judgments; see also Christensen 1983;
Zellner and Durlach 2003
).
It is worth noting that researchers who have documented
null resul ts of the addition of food coloring on ratings of
taste and/or flavor inte nsity have rarely bothered to conduct
any kind of analysis to ensure they had sufficient power in
their ex perimental design to observe a significant effect had
one been present (see Frick 1995). Because of the nature of
flavor research, the number of trials a given participant can
complete tends to be rather limited,
4
hence constraining the
ability of experimenters to detect what may well be a
relatively small behavioral effect (see also Lecoutre and Derzko
2001; Rouanet 1996). Complicating this issue even further is
the fact that the effects of food coloring on ratings of taste and
flavor intensity , or on the detection of the presence of a tastant
in solution, appear to be rather stimulus-specific (e.g., see
Bayarrietal.2001;Maga1974; Zampini et al. 2007, 2008).
Note that a similar stimulus specificity has also been reported
previously for the case of olfactorygustatory interactions
(e.g., see Frank and Byram 1988; though see also Valentin et
al. 2006). It is therefore currently unclear whether the discre-
pant results of color on taste intensity reporte d by dif ferent
groups of researchers over the last 50 years are best explained
in terms of a failure to properly control for all of the potential
confounding variables or by a lack of statistical power in the
experimental designs used by certain researchers in the field;
plausible arguments can be made in both directions.
Does Food Color Influe nce Perceived Flavor Identity?
By contrast, many (and, in fact, we would argue all) studies
published to date support the claim that judgments of flavor
identity are influenced by a foods color (e.g., DuBose et al.
1980; Hall 1958; Institute of Food Technologists 1980;
Kanig 1955; Levitan et al. 2008;Moir1936;Sakai2004;
Shankar et al. 2009;Stillman1993; Zampini et al. 2007,
2008; see also Dolnick 2008; note that we are aware of no
studies that have examined the effect of food color on taste
identification, and hence in this section, we only discuss
studies of colors influence on flavor identification). By
contrast, several studies have investigated the effect of color
on orthonasal olfactory odor identification judgments. So, for
example, Zellner et al. (1991) have shown that participants
correctly identify odors more rapidly when colored appro-
priately than when uncolored or else inappropriately colored.
4
Note here also that many of the demonstrations of the influence of color
on taste and/or flavor in the literature have actually been reported in those
studies in which the participants were only given a very small number of
stimuli to evaluate (e.g., five or less stimuli in certain of the experiments
reported by DuBose et al. 1980; Hoegg and Alba 2007b;Hyman1983;
Lavin and Lawless 1998;Morrotetal.2001;Orametal.1995; Stillman
1993;or615 stimuli in the studies reported by Alley and Alley 1998;
Shankar et al. 2009). This may be particularly important as people
appear to quickly learn that the color of a beverage no longer predicts a
particular taste (see Stevenson et al. 2000).
Chem. Percept. (2010) 3:6884 73
In terms of studies looking at colors effect on flavor
identification, DuBose et al. (1980) conducted one fre-
quently cited study addressing this question. The partic-
ipants in their study attempted to identify the flavors of a
variety of differently colored fruit-flavored drink s. Certain
colorflavor pairings were deemed appropriate (e.g., a
cherry-flavored drink colored red), while others were
deemed inappropriate (e.g., as when the lime-flavored
drink was colored red; though see Shankar et al. 2009 on
the notion of appropriateness). DuBos e et al. found that
participants misidentified the flavor of a number of the
drinks when the coloring was inappropriate. What is more,
participants incorrect answers often seemed to be driven by
the colors of the drinks themselves. That is, the participants
often made what could be classed as visually dominant
responses (see Partan and Marler 1999; Posner et al. 1976).
So, for example, 26% of the participants reported that a
cherry-flavored drink tasted of lemon/lime when colored
green as compared to no-lime-flavor responses when the
drink was colored red instead (see Table 3 for a summary of
the results from one of the experiments conducted by
DuBose et al.).
One potentially important limitation with regard to the
interpretation of the study of DuBose et al. (1980 ), and, in
fact, with the majority of other studies that have investi-
gated crossmodal influences of food coloring on flavor
perception in humans (e.g., Hall 1958; Kanig 1955; Moir
1936; Oram et al. 1995), is that the participants were not
informed of the deception that was taking place (e.g., see
Hall 1958, p. 229). This is a particularly important issue
given that it means that the participants in the majority of
the studies that have investigated the effect of color on
flavor identification may simply have assumed that the
colors of the drinks were meant to be informative with
regard to their likely flavor. In other words, if participants
found it difficult to discriminate the identity of the flavor
(or taste) on the basis of gustatory, olfactory, and/or flavor
cues, then they may simply have decided to respond on the
basis of the more easily discriminable color cues instead.
Hence, it is enti rely possible that the participants in these
studies may simply have used the salient visual cues as a
cognitive shortcut with regard to the likely flavors of the
foods that they were being asked to evaluate (cf. Bertelson
and Aschersleben 1998 for a similar confound that has been
identified in the literature documenting the visual capture,
or dominance, over perceived auditory localization). That
is, the participants in these earlier studies of multisensory
flavor perception may have felt some pressure to respond in
line with the salient information provided visually (namely,
the obviously changing color of the drink from one trial to
the next; cf. Orne 1962 ). As such, it is unclear whether the
results reported in many of the early studies demonstrating
colors influence on flavor identification responses reflect a
response bias elicited by the clearly visible (and changing)
colors of the drinks that were presented to the participants,
a genuine crossmodal perceptual effect (i.e., meaning that
the color cues actually modulated the flavor percept itself;
see Green and Swets 1966; Hoegg and Al ba 2007a), or
some unknown combination of these two effects (see below
for a fuller discu ssion of this issue).
5
One means by which researchers have attempted to
circumvent (or at least reduce) this potential uncertainty
regarding the cause of colors influence on flavor identifi-
cation responses is by explicitly informing their participants
of the potential deception prior to the start of the study (see
Stillman 1993; Zampini et al. 2007, 2008 ). Although only a
few studies have, to date, taken this precaution, the results
nevertheless confirm the suggestion that food color ing can
still affect peoples flavor identification responses under
such conditions. For example, the 310 untrained observers
(visitors to an open day at the University of Auckland) in a
study reported by Stillman (1993) were each given a
Table 3 Partial summary of the results from DuBose et al. (1980)
(experiment 2)
Reported flavor Color of drink
Red (%) Orange (%) Green (%)
Cherry 70 41 37
Orange 0 19 0
Lime 0 0 26
The results highlight the profound effect that food coloring can have
on participants flavor identification responses. The participants in this
study had to try and identify 16 different sequentially presented
beverages created by fully crossing the factors of flavor (cherry-,
orange-, or lime-flavored, or flavorless) and color (red, orange, green,
colorless). The participants were given a checklist of 14 possible
responses (including 12 fruit flavors) to choose from when trying to
identify each of the drinks (strawberry, raspberry, lemon, lime, grape,
apple, cherry, orange, blueberry, lemon lime, grapefruit, apricot, other,
or no flavor). The table highlights the distribution of responses from
the three most common flavor responses for the cherry-flavored drink.
The numerical values indicate the percentages of each flavor response
for each color
5
Note that we do not wish to argue that decisional biases in the
context of food colorings influence on multisensory flavor perception
are not, in and of themselves, interesting. They most certainly are. Our
point here is rather that the decisional biases that are elicited not
simply by the color of the foodstuff itself, but rather by the
ecologically invalid context in which the participants in these
laboratory studies often find themselves, may not be especially
informative (see also Garber et al. 2001, 2003 for a similar argument).
Our concern is that the results of such studies may say more about
how participants respond when placed in an ecologically invalid
laboratory context than they do about colors influence on flavor
perception in the real world.
74 Chem. Percept. (2010) 3:6884
beverage to taste and identify and were informed that its
color was independent of the flavor. The drinks were either
raspberry- or orange-flavored and were either colored red,
yellowish orange, green, or else left uncolored. The results
(see Fig. 2) showed that participants were significantly
better at correctly ident ifying the raspberry flavor when the
drink was colored red than when it was colored green,
orange yellow, or else was presented as a colorless solution.
The orange-flavored solution was identified significantly
more accurately when the drink was colored orange, yellow,
or red than when it was presented as a colorless solution.
The participants in another study reported by Zampini
et al. (2007; experiment 2) were also explicitl y informed
that there was no relationship between the color and flavor
of the drinks that they had to evalua te. That is, they were
informed that each of the 124 drinks that they had to
sample was just as likely to appear as a c olorless solution,
or as a red, green, orange, yellow, blue, or gray solution,
regardless of its actual flavor. Even though Zampini et al.s
participants were clearly aware that the colors of the
drinks that they were tasting were not in any way
informative (cf. Engen 1972), the colors of the drinks
nevertheless still exerted a significan t influence on parti-
cipants flavor identification responses (see Fig. 3). Similar
results were also reported in a follow-up study (Zampini
et al. 2008) in which the participants had to try and identify
the f lavor of blackcurrant, orange, and flavorless solutions
that had been colored yellow, gray, orange, red, or else left
colorless (see Fig. 4). Taken together, the results of
Zampini et al. s(2007, 2008) studies therefore add weight
to the cla im that the crossmoda l effect of color on multi-
sensory f lavor perception in humans does not reflect
task demands or any simple form of decisional bias (cf.
Delwiche 2004;HoeggandAlba2007b).
Interim Summary
In summary, then, many studies over the last 70 years or so
have provided empirical support for the claim that the color
of a food/drink can exert a powerful influence on peoples
flavor identification responses (e.g., DuBose et al. 1980;
Hall 1958; Kanig 1955; Levitan et al. 2008; Moir 1936;
Oram et al. 1995; Shankar et al. 2009; Stillman 1993;
Zampini et al. 2007, 2008). What is more, we are not aware
of any studies that have reported results that conflict with
this claimi.e., where the color of a food was (noticeably)
changed but where this change had no effect on partic-
ipants flavor identification responses.
6
As such, we would
argue that Lavin and Lawlesss(1998, p. 284) claim that
the literature on the effects of color on taste and flavour
judgments is consistent in its inconsistency is incorrect for
at least as far as the effects of food coloring on peoples
flavor identification responses are concerned, the answer
appears clear-cut: food coloring most certainly does
influence peoples flavor identification responses.
Here, though, it is also important to bear in mind that
one does not tend to see complete visual dominance
(sometimes known as capture). That is, participants
flavor identification responses do not always match the color
that they see. Instead, researchers typically report a visual
biasing of participants responses on a certain proportion of
the trials, with the exact proportion varying from one study
to the next. So, for example, DuBose et al. (1980)showed
that atypically colored drinks were identified by their color
on 40% of the trials and by their flavor on 28% of the trials.
Meanwhile, the participants in Stillmans(1993) study were
able to correctly identify the flavor of the drink in 60% of
trials. Oram et al. (1995) reported that for adults tested in
their study, responses were based on color on 13% of trials
and on flavor on 79% of trials. We discuss possible reasons
for this discrepancy below.
Perceptual vs. Decisional Contributions to Colors
Influence on Flavor Identification
None of the studies reported thus far bear directly (and
unequivocally) on the question of whether these cross modal
effects on participants flavor identification responses
reflect a decisional effect, a perceptual effect, or some
1
*
*
0.8
0.6
Uncoloured
0.4
Red
Orange
0.2
Green
0
Proportion correct
Raspberry Orange
Flavour of the solution
Fig. 2 Summary of the results of Stillmans(1993) study in which
310 participants were given a single drink to taste and identify. The
horizontal lines indicate those comparisons between conditions that
were significant (asterisk) using independent samples chi-square tests
6
The one exception to this claim comes from one of the conditions in
Zampini et al.s(2007, Experiment 2) study in which participants
flavor identification responses for the strawberry-flavored solutions
were not significantly affected by the changes that were introduced
into the colors of the drinks. However, given that the numerical trends
were in the appropriate direction, this null result may simply reflect a
lack of statistical power.
Chem. Percept. (2010) 3:6884 75
unknown combination of the two. One problem here in
terms of trying to answer this question is that there are no
simple means of assessing perceptual sensitivity (such as
the d measure that is afforded by signal detection theory;
Green and Swets 1966) when participants are allowed to
make unconstrained flavor identification responses, as often
happens when one is trying to identify a foods flavor in
natural settings. While it is certainly true that techniques do
exist to try and counteract the contribution of any guessing
biases that may be present (see Chen and Spence 2010 ;
Intraub 1984), no one has as yet used them in order to try
and isolate any genuinely perceptual contribution to colors
influence on participants flavor identification responses
(see also OMahony 1992).
As such, all that can be said with any confidence at the
present time is that all of the studies that have been
published to date (no matter whether using a free respond-
ing or forced choice discrimination task) have shown a
100
LIME FLAVOR
a)
7575
5050
Correct responses (%)
25
0
Colorless
Green-
standard
Green-
double
Orange-
standard
Orange-
double
Red-
standard
Red-
double
Color of the solutions
100
ORANGE FLAVOR
b)
100
7575
50
25
Correct responses (%)
0
Colorless
Green- Green-
double
Orange-
standard
Orange-
double
Red-
standard
Red-
doublestandard double standard double standard double
Color of the solutions
Fig. 3 Summary of the results of the study of Zampini et al (2007)
(experiment 2) documenting the effect of color on flavor identifica-
tion. Lime, orange, strawberry, and flavorless drinks were presented
either colored green, orange, red, or colorless in a fully counter-
balanced design. Either a standard or double intensity of the colorant
was added to the solutions. The participants had to try and identify the
flavor of each drink from a list of 22 alternatives (if the participants
indicated the other option they were prompted to suggest the
specific flavor they had in mind) and also rate the flavor intensity.
They were given 124 samples to evaluate in total using a sip-and-spit
method. Coloring the solutions congruently (e.g., green for lime, red
for strawberry, and orange for orange) led to a modest increase in the
number of correct flavor identification responses for the strawberry-
and orange-flavored drinks (relative to the performance seen when
participants judged the colorless solutions). Incongruent coloring
impaired the participants ability to identify the orange- (when colored
green or red) and lime-flavored solution (when colored orange or red).
Overall, color appeared to have less of an impact on the identification
of the strawberry solutions. At present, there is no obvious explanation
for this particular result. Note also that it is difficult to discern any
clear pattern of results from the doubling of the intensity of the food
coloring added to the various solutions
76 Chem. Percept. (2010) 3:6884
crossmodal effect of color on participants flavor identifi-
cation responses. Whether this reflects a genuine perceptual
effect, a predom inantly decisional effect, or some unknown
combination of the two effects will only be resolved by
future research. Progress here will likely come through the
use of more sophisticated psychophysical paradigms (e.g.,
see Odgaard et al. 2003 for such an approach being applied
to the auditory modulation of visual brightness judgments;
see also Lau et al. 1995). It is also possible that a definitive
answer to the question of whether or not colors crossmodal
influence on flavor identification occurs at a perceptual
level may be assisted by the evidence emerging from
cognitive neuroscience research. It would, for example, be
interesting to know whether the neural activity in primary
(or possibly secondary) gustatory cortex and perhaps more
interestingly in orbitofrontal cortex elicited by a gusta tory/
olfactory/flavor stimulus ca n be chan ged simply b y
changing a food or drinks color (cf. González et al. 2006;
McClure et al. 2004; Österbauer et al. 2005; Small 2004;
Veldhuizen et al. 2009; Verhagen and Engelen 2006). Such
effects, sho uld they be observed, would certainly be
consistent with the existence of at least some perceptual
component to the crossmodal effect of color on flavor
perception. Given that such top-down effects have recently
been observed on the basis of verbal food/taste descriptors
(e.g., Nitschke et al. 2006; Sarinopoulos et al. 2006), we are
optimistic that they might also be found here (that is, for the
case of visual color cues).
Expectancy-Based Effects of Food Coloring
It seems likely that whenever we see a food of a certain
color, that color, together with any other contextual cues (e.g.,
100
STRAWBERRY FLAVOR
c)
7575
5050
25
Correct responses (%)
0
Colorless
Green-
standard
Green-
double
Orange-
standard
Orange-
double
Red-
standard
Red-
double
Color of the solutions
100
FLAVORLESS
d)
100
7575
50
25
0
Colorless
Green- Green-
double
Orange-
standard
Orange-
double
Red-
standard
Red-
doublestandard double standard double standard double
Color of the solutions
Correct responses (%)
Fig. 3 (continued)
Chem. Percept. (2010) 3:6884 77
is it a food or drink item, hot or cold, transparent or opaque,
etc; what Francis 1977, describes as visual appearance
cues), will lead us to generate specific expectations regarding
the likely flavor of that food (see Cardello 2007;Kochand
Koch 2003; Lee et al. 2006; Levitan et al. 2008;Shankaret
al. 2009; Yeomans et al. 2008; Zellner and Durlach 2003).
Zampini et al. (2007) (experiment 1) recently investigated
the nature of these crossmodal associations by showing
participants (all from the UK and of UK origin) a range of
colored drinks (green, orange, yellow, blue, gray, red, and
colorless) and asking them to look at each drink in turn and
simply report (without tasting) what flavor they would
expect it to have. The results (see Table 4)showedthatall
of the colored drinks generated systematic expectations
regarding a drinks likely flavor. It is our belief that such
color-induced flavor expectations (what Hutchings 1977 called
anticipatory effects) may lead to the misidentification of
flavors when a drink is subsequently tasted (cf. Stevenson and
Oaten 2008). Misidentification at this (cognitive) level will
presumably also provide access to a wealth of semantic
information about the misidentified flavor (see Engen
1972; Gottfried and Dolan 2003; Jönsson et al. 2005;
Marques 2006;Morrotetal.2001;Parretal.2002; Revonsuo
1999; Skrandies and Reuther 2008; Zellner et al. 1991),
which in turn might reasonably be expected to influence
participants judgments/responses. This misidentification
may then have perceptual (i.e., as well as decisional)
consequences (see also Williams et al. 1984a, b). Here, one
need only think of the placebo effect where a personsbeliefs
(be they induced by the color of a pill or verbal labeling)
have bee n shown to have surprisingly low-level (i.e.,
perceptual) effects (de Craen et al. 1996; Plassmann et al.
2008;Shivetal.2005).
Regarding the expectancy-based account of color
s
influence on multisensory flavor perception, it is interesting
to note that red, one of the colors that have often been shown
to generate strong expectations concerning a foodstaste,
odor, and flavor (e.g., Koch and Koch 2003;OMahony
1983; see also Demattè et al. 2006), also happens to be one
of the colors that are most often reported as having an effect
in terms of influencing flavor, taste, and odor perception (in
terms of both identity and intensity judgments; e.g., Hyman
1983; Johnson and Clydesdale 1982;Maga1974;Morrotet
al. 2001;Orametal.1995; Stillman 1993;seeFig.2). It
should, however, also be noted that different colors will
likely lead to the generation of different taste/flavor expecta-
tions by different people as a function of their background
and/or culture (Shankar et al. 2009; see also Scanlon 1985),
expertise/experience with tasting a particular foodstuff
(e.g., Pangborn et al. 1963; Parr et al. 2003; Smith 2007;
Urbányi 1982; Williams et al. 1984a, b; though see also
Table 4 Results of the study of Zampini et al. (2007) (experiment 1)
asse ssing the flavor expectations generated by a group of UK
participants on being presented with clear plastic beakers containing
transparent drinks that had been colored green, orange, yellow, blue,
gray, red, or left colorless
Color of
drink
Expected flavor (% of participants with that
expectation)
Green Lime (69%), apple (20%), melon (11%)
Orange Orange (91%), aniseed (5%), toffee (4%)
Yellow Lemon (89%), pear (5%), apple (4%), melon (2%)
Blue Spearmint (86%), raspberry (9%), cream soda (5%)
Gray Blackcurrant (53%), licorice (40%), cherry (4%),
aniseed (4%)
Red Strawberry (46%), raspberry (27%), cherry (27%)
Colorless Flavorless (51%), cream soda (16%), vanilla (15%),
aniseed (15%), spearmint (2%), melon (2%), pear (2%)
The table shows the percentage of trials in which participants reported
that they expected a particular colored drink to taste of a certain flavor
100
Orange
75
50
25
Correct responses (%)
0
Yellow Grey Orange Red Colorless
Color of the solution
100
Blackcurrant
7575
50
25
00
Yellow Grey Orange Red Colorless
100
Flavorless
75
50
25
0
Yellow Grey Orange Red Colorless
Correct responses (%)Correct responses (%)
Color of the solution
Color of the solution
Fig. 4 Summary of the results of the study of Zampini et al. (2008)
highlighting the crossmodal effect of color on participants flavor
identification responses. Once again, a sip-and-spit method was used
78 Chem. Percept. (2010) 3:6884
Teerling 1992), and age (Oram et al. 1995; Philipsen et al.
1995; see also Lavin and Lawless 1998).
Shankar et al. (2009) recently investigated the nature of
any cross-cultural differences in colorflavor expectancy
effects. The two groups of participants in their study, one
from the UK the other from Taiwan, were shown seven
colored drinks (brown, blue, yellow, orange, green, clear,
and red) and had to report on the flavor that they expected
each colored drink to taste of. Just as in the study of
Zampini et al. (2007), the participants in each group were
found to generate consistent flavor expectations based on
the colors of the drinks. Importantly, however, the flavor
expectations generated by the two groups of participants
were significantly different for several (but importantly, not
all) of the colored drinks (see Table 5).
Given the individual differen ces in the fl avo r expect-
ations generated by viewing a foods col oring, a critical
experiment t hat has yet to be conducted would involve
investigating whether the flavor expectations held by an
individual do indeed lea d to the misi dentific ation of a
food or drink s flavor or aroma. Our prediction is that if
one p erson expects a brown drink to taste of cola while
another expects a d rink of the same color to ta ste of grape
(see Ta ble 5; see also Oram et al. 19 95), then adding a
brown color to a drink should result in different flavor
(aroma) identification responses for thes e two individu-
als. While th is experiment sounds (and indeed is) ve ry
simple, no o ne has, as yet, me asured their participants
flavor (or for that matter taste or odor) expectations and
then followed through to investigate whether those
expectations do indeed predict the subsequent pattern of
flavor (t aste or odor ) misidentification that results when
that color is added to a foodstuff (cf. Cardello 200 7;
Shankar et al., submitted).
While some researchers appear to believe that such
cognitive effects of color on flavor will not have any low-
level (e.g., physiological/perceptual) consequences (e.g.,
see Keast et al. 2004; Fig. 1 ), others clearly think that
colors influence on flavor is perceptual in nature (e.g., see
Delwiche 2004;Fig.1). Discussion here though is
complicated by the fact that different researchers appear to
use the same terms (e.g., perceptual) to mean very
different things. For example, while Delwiche (2004)
(Fig. 1) draws a clear distinction between cognitive and
perceptual effects, others appear to view cognitive/expecta-
tion effects as being perceptual (as opposed to physical,
sensory, or hedonic; see Cardello 1996; Fig. 1). It is
therefore important to note that expectancy effects (here
one can also think of labeling, pricing, and/or branding as
forms of expectancy effect; e.g., Cardello 1994, 2007;
Hutchings
1977; Shiv et al. 2005; though see Davis 1981)
have been shown in neuroimaging studies to have surpris-
ingly early effects on sensory information processing in
humans (e.g., McClure et al. 2004; Nitschke et al. 2006;
Plassmann et al. 2008; Sarinopoulos et al. 2006). Such
results therefore raise the possibility that color-induced
expectancy effects might also influence a persons percep-
tual representation of flavor information and not just the
response that they happen to make.
Color-induced expectancy effects might influence flavor
perception (in particular, identification) by directing a
participants attention to a salient component of a flavor
stimulus (e.g., Ashkenazi and Marks 2004; Marks 2002;
Marks and Wheeler 1998; Marshall et al. 2006; Oram et al.
1995). Research elsewhere in psychology has shown that
the focusing of a participants attention on a particular
stimulus or stimulus attribute can enhance the perceptual
representation of that stimulus or stimulus attribut e (i.e.,
can have an effect on perceptual sensitivity as measured by
a change in d). It is, though, important to bear in mind that
color cues can (and do) sometimes lead to decisional effects
as well (e.g., by influencing peoples criteria for making
various judgments; cf. Engen 1972; McCullough et al.
1978). Such decisional effects are likely to be especially
prominent when the stimulus itself is ambiguous (as flavor
stimuli often are; cf. Chaiken 1980; Wilson and Klaaren
1992). Taken together, the research reviewed in this section
therefore supports the view that higher level (or top-
down) cognitive beliefs or expectations can (and do) have
low-level (early) effects.
The Multisensory Integration of Color/Flavor Cues
One of the most important issues for future research
concerns the question of whether in addition to any top-
down cognitive influence (in the form of expectancy
effects), food coloring can also influence flavor identifica-
tion in humans (automatically) in a b ottom-up manner via
multisensory integration. The mul tisensory integration of
flavor cues in a food or drink with an atypical visual color
cue would be expected to lead to a form of sensory (i.e.,
visual) dominance over flavor perception (see Ernst and
Banks 2002; Ernst and Bülthoff 2004; Posner et al. 1976;
Spence 2008). Numerous neurophysiological studies have
already demonstrated the integration of olfactory, gustatory,
textural, and visual cues in the orbitofrontal cortex of both
monkey and man (see Gottfried and Dolan 2003; Österbauer
et al. 2005; Rolls and Baylis 1994; Rolls et al. 1996). Given
that the Bayesian approach is currently proving so
successful elsewhere in the study of multisensory integra-
tion, one might expect this approach to understanding
multisensory perception to have an influence here as well
soon. However, the problem that one soon runs into in
terms of trying to separate top-down (i.e., cognitive) and
bottom-up (i.e., stimulus-driven) influences is that many of
Chem. Percept. (2010) 3:6884 79
the factors that are likely to promote top-down cognitive
effects are also likely to increase the likelihood of bottom-
up multisensory integration as wel l (see Spence 2007 on
this point).
Isolating the Bottom-Up Effects of Color on Flavor
Perception
In the future, though, it may be possible to isolate any
bottom-up effects of color on flavor perception by utilizing
articulatory suppression to minimize the effect of top-down
cognitive facto rs on participants performance. For in-
stance, Stevenson and Oaten (2008) recently used this
technique in order to investigate whether colors effect on
orthonasal olfaction is automatic or not. The cherry and
strawberry olfactants used in this study were dissolved in
liquids that were colored green, red, or else left colorless.
Stevenson and Oaten demonstrated that the crossmodal
influence of color (either appropriate or inappropriate) on
odor discrimination performance was significantly reduced
when their participants engaged in articulatory suppression
(saying the word the out loud repeatedly) while evaluat-
ing the odors. This result suggests that the crossmodal
effect of color on odor perception is not mandatory (or, in
Stevenson and Oatens words, automatic). As such, it
provides some evidence against color influencing odor
perception through a process of bottom-up mul tisensory
integration (perhaps leading to sensory dominance) since
research in other areas of psychology has shown that
multisensory integration tends to be relatively automatic
(i.e., unaffected by the performance of a secondary task;
see Navarra et al. 2009; Santangelo and Spence 2008 for
reviews).
It would, of course, be ideal to utilize the articulatory
suppression technique in order to assess whether one can
eliminate (or at least modulate) colors crossmodal effect on
flavor identification.
7
If one were to obtain a similar result
to that reported by Stevenson and Oaten (2008), it would
provide eviden ce that colors influence on flavor perception
is mediated primari ly by cognitive (or top-down) expectan-
cy factors and not by bottom-up (i.e., automatic) multisen-
sory integration. One might think such a result likely given
Stevenson and Oatens use of food odors. However, it is
important to note that Koza et al. (2005) have shown that
color can have qualitatively different effects on olfactory
perception as a function of whether an odor is delivered
orthonasally (as in Stevenson and Oatens study) or
retronasally (as when actually consuming a food or drink).
Alternatively, howe ver, one may also be able to dissociate
Table 5 Results of the cross-cultural study of Shankar et al. (2010) assessing the flavor expectations elicited by differently colored drinks in two
groups of participants, one from the UK, the other from Taiwan
The table highlights the three most commonly expected flavor responses (chosen by more than one participant). The shaded rows indicate the
colored drinks (brown, blue, yellow, and orange) that elicited significantly different flavor expectations from the two groups of participants
7
Though, of course, there may be some practical challenges
associated with trying to taste a colored solution while at the time
speaking out loud. One could though instead ask the participant to
read out a list of words silently in their head or else to engage in some
other highly attention-demanding task, for example, monitoring a
rapid serial visual presentation stream for occasionally presented
targets (e.g., see Santangelo and Spence 2008).
80 Chem. Percept. (2010) 3:6884
bottom-up multisensory integration effects from top-down
effects resul ting from cognitive expectancies by playing
with the parameters of stimulus presentation, such as by
varying the timing and/or spatial relationship between the
color and the flavor (see also Lee et al. 2006). Bottom-up
integration tends to be more sensitive to spatiotemporal
coincidence of the constituent signals than are top-down
cognitive effects (see Spence 2007).
It may, of course, turn out that the crossmodal effect of
food coloring on flavo r identification operates in both a top-
down manner via cognitive expectations and in a bottom-up
manner via the sensory dominance resulting from the
automatic (i.e., involuntary) multisensory integration of
the various unimodal sensory signals. Complicating matters
further here though is the fact that the relative contribution
of top-down and bottom-up factors might also vary as a
function of the speci fic combinations of color (and other
visual appearance cues; Francis 1977; Levitan et al. 2008)
and flavor being investigated. On the one hand, the more
that a color is associated with a particular taste or flavor
(such as red being associated with sweetness in ripe fruits
and green with tartness/sourness in unripe fruits; Maga
1974; Pangborn 1960), the stronger one might expect that
both the bottom-up and top-down effects should be. By
contrast, the consequences of marketing, for example, the
recent introduction of the association between blue coloring
and raspberry flavoring (see Garber et al. 2008; Shankar et
al. 2010; see also Triplett 1994), might, at least initially, be
expected to have a primarily cognitive effect.
Having demonstrated that food coloring does indeed
influence flavor identification, the question of how best to
unravel the various mechanism(s), at both a behavioral and
neural level, underlying this crossmodal effect clearly
represents an important and challenging area for study in
the years to come. Part of the challenge here comes from
the fact that (as shown in Fig. 1) it is currently unclear
whether colors crossmodal influence on flavor perception
in any given situation is being driven by colors influence
on taste, smell (orthonasal or retronasal), or directly on the
flavor percept itself. What is, though, certain at the present
time is that color plays an integral part in our experience of,
and responses toward, food and drink even if, as suggested
by the ISO (1992, 2008), visual cues do not constitute a
core attribute of flavor perception.
References
Alley RL, Alley TR (1998) The influence of physical state and color
on perceived sweetness. J Psychology: Interdisciplinary Applied
132:561568
Ashkenazi A, Marks LE (2004) Effect of endogenous attention on
detection of weak gustatory and olfactory flavors. Percept
Psychophys 66:596608
Auvray M, Spence C (2008) The multisensory perception of flavor.
Conscious Cogn 17:10161031
Bartoshuk LM (1975) Taste mixtures: is mixture suppression related
to compression? Physiol Behav 14:643649
Bayarri S, Calvo C, Costell E, Duran L (2001) Influence of color on
perception of sweetness and fruit flavor of fruit drinks. Food Sci
Technol Int 7:399404
Bertelson P, Asch ersleben G (1998) Automatic visual bias of
perceived auditory location. Psychon Bull Rev 5:482489
Blackwell L (1995) Visual clues and their effects on odour
assessment. Nutr Food Sci 5:2428
Brice BA (1954) Color and its relationship to food investigations. In
Farrell KT, Wagner JR, Peterson MS, MacKinney G (eds) Color
in foods: A symposium sponsored by the Quartermaster Food
and Container Institute for the Armed Forces Quartermaster
Research and Development Command U. S. Army Quartermaster
Corps. Na tional Academy of Sciences - National Research
Council: Washington, pp. 416
Cain WS (1979) To know with the nose: keys to odor identification.
Science 203:467470
Cardello AV (1994) Consumer expectations and their role in food
acceptance. In: MacFie HJH, Thomson DMH (eds) Measurement
of food preferences. Blackie Academic & Professional, London,
pp 253297
Cardello AV (1996) The role of the human senses in food acceptance.
In: Meiselman HL, McFie HJH (eds) Food choice, acceptance
and consumption. Blackie Academic & Professional, New York,
pp 182
Cardello AV (2007) Measuring consumer expectations to improve food
product development. In: MacFie HJH (ed) Co nsumer-led food
product development. Woodhead Publishing, Cambridge, pp 223261
Chaiken S (1980) Heuristic vs systematic information processing and
the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. J Pers Soc
Psychol 29:752766
Chan MM, Kane-Martinelli C (1997) The effect of color on perceived
flavor intensity and acceptance of foods by young adults and
elderly adults. J Am Diet Assoc 97:657659
Chen Y-C, Spence C (2010) When hearing the bark helps to identify
the dog: semantically-congruent sounds modulate the identifica-
tion of masked pictures. Cognition 114:389404
Christensen C (1983) Effects of color on aroma, flavor and texture
judgments of foods. J Food Sci 48:787790
Christensen C (1985) Effect of color on judgments of food aroma and
food intensity in young and elderly adults. Perception 14:755762
Davis RG (1981) The role of nonolfactory context cues in odor
identification. Percept Psychophys 30:8389
de Craen AJ, Roos PJ, Leonard de Vries A, Kleijnen J (1996) Effect of
colour of drugs: systematic review of perceived effect of drugs
and of their effectiveness. Br Med J 313:16241626
de Wijk RA, Polet IA, Engelen L et al (2004) Amount of ingested
custard dessert as affected by its color, odor, and texture. Physiol
Behav 82:397403
Delwiche J (2004) The impact of perceptual interactions on perceived
flavor. Food Qual Prefer 15:137146
Demattè ML, Sanabria D, Spence C (2006) Cross-modal associations
between odors and colors. Chem Senses 31:531538
Desor J, Beauchamp G (1974) The human capacity to transmit
olfactory information. Percept Psychophys 16:551556
Dolnick E (2008) Fish or foul? The New York Times, September 2.
Downloaded from http://www.nytimes.com/2008/09/02/opinion/
02dolnick.html?_r=1&scp=1&sq=chocolate%20strawberry%
20yogurt&st=cse on 07/07/09
DuBose CN, Cardello AV, Maller O (1980) Effects of colorants and
flavorants on identification, perceived flavor intensity, and
hedonic quality of fruit-flavored beverages and cake. J Food
Sci 45:13931399, 1415
Chem. Percept. (2010) 3:6884 81
Duncker K (1939) The influence of past experience upon perceptual
properties. Am J Psychol 52:255265
Engen T (1972) The effect of expectation on judgments of odour. Acta
Psychol 36:450458
Ernst MO, Banks MS (2002) Humans integrate visual and haptic
information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415:429433
Ernst MO, Bülthoff HH (2004) Merging the senses into a robust
percept. Trends Cogn Sci 8:162169
Eskenazi B, Cain WS, Friend K (1986) Exploration of olfactory
aptitude. Bull Psychon Soc 24:203206
Fletcher L, Heymann H, Ellersieck M (1991) Effects of visual
masking techniques on the intensity rating of sweetness of
gelatins and lemonades. J Sens Stud 6:179191
Francis FJ (1977) Color and appearance as dominating sensory
properties of food. In: Birch GG, Brennan JG, Parker KJ (eds)
Sensory properties of foods. Applied Science, Barking, pp 4547
Frank RA, Byram J (1988) Tastesmell interactions are tastant and
odorant dependent. Chem Senses 13:445455
Frank R, Ducheny K, Mize S (1989) Strawberry odor, but not red
color, enhances the sweetness of sucrose solutions. Chem Senses
14:371377
Frick RW (1995) Accepting the null hypothesis. Mem Cogn 23:132138
Frost MB, Janhoj T (2007) Understanding creaminess. Int Dairy J
17:12981311
Garber LL Jr, Hyatt EM, Starr RG Jr (2001) Placing food color
experimentation into a valid consumer context. J Food Prod
Mark 7(3):3 24
Garber LL Jr, Hyatt EM, Starr RG Jr (2003) Measuring consumer
response to food products. Food Qual Prefer 14:315
Garber LL Jr, Hyatt EM, Boya ÜÖ (2008) The mediating effects of
the appearance of nondurable consumer goods and their
packaging on consumer behavior. In: Schifferstein HNJ, Hekkert
P (eds) Product experience. Elsevier, London, pp 581602
Gifford SR, Clydesdale FM (1986) The psychophysical relationship
between color and sodium chloride concentrations in model
systems. J Food Prot 49:977982
Gifford SR, Clydesdale FM, Damon RA Jr (1987) The psychophysical
relationship between color and salt concentrations in chicken
flavored broths. J Sens Stud 2:137147
González J, Barros-Loscertales A, Pulvermü ller F, Meseguer V,
Sanjuán A, Belloch V et al (2006) Reading cinnamon activates
olfactory brain regions. NeuroImage 32:906912
Gottfried JA, Dolan RJ (2003) The nose smells what the eye sees:
crossmodal visual facilitation of human olfactory perception.
Neuron 39:375386
Green DM, Swets JA (1966) Signal detection theory and psychophy-
sics. Wiley, New York
Hall RL (1958) Flavor study approaches at McCormick and Company,
Inc. In: A. D. Little Inc. (ed) Flavor research and food
acceptance: a survey of the scope of flavor and associated
research, compiled from papers presented in a series of symposia
given in 19561957. Reinhold, New York, pp 224240
Hoegg J, Alba JW (2007a) Linguistic framing of sensory experience:
there is some accounting for taste. In: Lowrey TM (ed)
Psycholinguistic phenomena in marketing communications.
Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 321
Hoegg J, Alba JW (2007b) Taste perception: more than meets the
tongue. J Consum Res 33:490
498
Hutchings JB (1977) The importance of visual appearance of foods to
the food processor and the consumer. In: Birch GG, Brennan JG,
Parker KJ (eds) Sensory properties of foods. Applied Science
Publishers, London, pp 4557
Hyman A (1983) The influence of color on the taste perception of
carbonated water preparations. Bull Psychon Soc 21:145148
Institute of Food Technologists (1980) Food colors. J Food Technol 34
(7):7784
Intraub H (1984) Conceptual masking: the effects of subsequent visual
events on memory for pictures. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn
10:115125
ISO (1992) Standard 5492: terms relating to sensory analysis.
International Organization for Standardization
ISO (2008) Standard 5492: terms relating to sensory analysis.
International Organization for Standardization
Johnson J (1982) Psychophysical relationships between colour and
sweetness in fruit flavored solutions. Ph.D. thesis. University of
Massachusetts at Amherst
Johnson J, Clydesdale FM (1982) Perceived sweetness and redness in
colored sucrose solutions. J Food Sci 47:747752
Johnson JL, Dzendolet E, Damon R, Sawyer M, Clydesdale FM (1982)
Psychophysical relationships between perceived sweetness and
color in cherry-flavored beverages. J Food Prot 45:601606
Johnson JL, Dzendolet E, Clydesdale FM (1983) Psychophysical
relationships between perceived sweetnes s and redne ss in
strawberry-flavored beverages. J Food Prot 46:2125, 28
Jönsson FU, Tchekhova A, Lönner P, Olsson MJ (2005) A
metamemory perspective on odor naming and identification.
Chem Senses 30:353365
Kanig JL (1955) Mental impact of colors in foods studied. Food Field
Reporter 23:57
Keast RSJ, Dalton PH, Breslin PAS (2004) Flavour interactions at the
sensory level. In: Taylor AJ, R oberts DD (eds) Flavour
perception. Blackwell Scientific, Oxford, pp 228255
Koch C, Koch EC (2003) Preconceptions of taste based on color. J
Psychol: Interdisciplinary Applied 137:233242
Kostyla AS (1978) The psychophysical relationships between color
and flavor of some fruit flavored beverages. PhD thesis,
University of Massachusetts, Amherst
Koza BJ, Cilmi A, Dolese M, Zellner DA (2005) Color enhances
orthonasal olfactory intensity and reduces retronasal olfactory
intensity. Chem Senses 30:643649
Laing DG, Link C, Jinks AL, Hutchinson I (2002) The limited
capacity of humans to identify the components of taste mixtures
and tasteodour mixtures. Perception 31:617635
Lau K-N, Post G, Kagan A (1995) Using economic incentives to
distinguish perception bias from discrimination ability in taste
tests. J Mark Res 32:140151
Lavin J, Lawless H (1998) Effects of color and odor on judgments of
sweetness among children and adults. Food Qual Prefer 9:283
289
Lecoutre B, Derzko G (2001) Asserting the smallness of effects in
ANOVA. Methods Psychological Research 6:132
Lee L, Frederick S, Ariely D (2006) Try it, youll like it: the influence
of expectation, consumption, and revelation on preferences for
beer. Psychol Sci 17:10541058
Levitan C, Zampini M, Li R, Spence C (2008) Assessing the role of
color cues and peoples beliefs about colorflavor associations on
the discrimination of the flavor of sugar-coated chocolates. Chem
Senses 33:415423
Maga JA (1974) Influence of color on taste thresholds. Chem Senses
Flavor 1:115119
Marks LE (2002) The role of attention in chemosensation. Food Qual
Prefer 14:147155
Marks LE, Wheeler ME (1998) Attention and the detectability of
weak taste stimuli. Chem Senses 23:1929
Marques JF (2006) Specialization and semantic organization: evidence
for multiple semantics linked to sensory modalities. Mem Cogn
34:6067
Marshall K, Laing DG, Jinks AL, Hutchinson I (2006) The capacity of
humans to identify components in complex odortaste mixtures.
Chem Senses 31:539545
Masurovsky BI (1939) How to obtain the right food color. Food Eng
11:13, 5556
82 Chem. Percept. (2010) 3:6884
McClure SM, Li J, Tomlin D, Cypert KS, Montague LM, Montague
PR (2004) Neural correlates of behavioral preference for
culturally familiar drinks. Neuron 44:379387
McCullough JM, Martinsen CS, Moinpour R (1978) Application of
multidimensional scaling to the analysis of sensory evaluations
of stimuli with known attribute structures. J Appl Psychol
63:103109
Moir HC (1936) Some observations on the appreciation of flavour in
foodstuffs. J Soc Chem Ind: Chemistry Industry Review 14:145148
Morrot G, Brochet F, Dubourdieu D (2001) The color of odors. Brain
Lang 79:309320
Navarra J, Alsius A, Soto-Faraco S, Spence C (2009) Assessing the
role of attention in the audiovisual integration of speech. Inf
Fusion 11:411
Nitschke JB, Dixon GE, Sarinopoulos I, Short SJ, Cohen JD, Smith
EE et al (2006) Altering expectancy dampens neural response to
aversive taste in primary taste cortex. Nat Neurosci 9:435442
Norton WE, Johnson FN (1987) The influence of intensity of colour
on perceived flavour characteristics. Med Sci Res 15:329330
OMahony M (1983) Gustatory responses to nongustatory stimuli.
Perception 12:627 633
OMahony M (1992) Understand ing discrimination tests: a user-
friendly treatment of response bias, rating and ranking R-index
texts and their relationship to signal detection. J Sens Stud 7:147
Odgaard EC, Arieh Y, Marks LE (2003) Cross-modal enhancement of
perceived brightness: sensory interaction versus response bias.
Percept Psychophys 65:123132
Oram N, Laing DG, Hutchinson I, Owen J, Rose G, Freeman M, Newell
G (1995) The influence of flavor and color on drink identification
by children and adults. Dev Psychobiol 28:239246
Orne MT (1962) On the social psychology of the psychological
experiment: with particular reference to demand characteristics
and their implications. Am Psychol 17:776783
Österbauer RA, Matthews PM, Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Hansen
PC, Calvert GA (2005) Color of scents: chromatic stimuli
modulate odor responses in the human brain. J Neurophysiol
93:34343441
Pangborn RM (1960) Influence of color on the discrimination of
sweetness. Am J Psychol 73:229238
Pangborn RM, Hansen B (1963) The influence of color on
discrimination of sweetness and sourness in pear-nectar. Am J
Psychol 76:315317
Pangborn RM, Berg HW, Hansen B (1963) The influence of color on
discrimination of sweetness in dry table-wine. Am J Psychol
76:492495
Parr WV, Heatherbell D, White KG (2002) Demystifying wine expertise:
olfactory threshold, perceptual skill and semantic memory in expert
and novice wine judges. Chem Senses 27:747755
Parr WV, White KG, Heatherbell D (2003) The nose knows: influence
of colour on perception of wine aroma. J Wine Res 14:79101
Partan S, Marler P (1999) Communication goes multimodal. Science
283:12721273
Philipsen DH, Clydesdale FM, Griffin RW, Stern P (1995) Consumer
age affects response to sensory character istics of a cherry
flavoured beverage. J Food Sci 60:364368
Plassmann H, ODoherty J, Shiv B, Rangel A (2008) Marketing
actions can modulate neural representations of experienced
pleasantness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:10501054
Posner MI, Nissen MJ, Klein RM (1976) Visual dominance: an
information-processing account of its origins and significance.
Psychol Rev 83:157171
Revonsuo A (1999) Binding and the phenomenal unity of conscious-
ness. Conscious Cogn 8:173185
Rolls ET, Baylis LL (1994) Gustatory, olfactory, and visual conver-
gence within the primate orbi tofrontal cortex. J Neurosci
14:54375452
Rolls ET, Critchley HD, Mason R, Wakeman EA (1996) Orbitofrontal
cortex neurons: role in olfactory and visual association learning. J
Neurophysiol 75:19701981
Romeu JV, De V icente MLS (1968) Influyen los colores en el sabor?
[Do colours influence flavour?]. Rev Interam Psicologia 2:143157
Roth HA, Radle LJ, Gifford SR, Clydesdale FM (1988) Psychophysical
relationships between perceived sweetness and color in lemon-
and lime-flavored drinks. J Food Sci 53(11161119):1162
Rouanet H (1996) Bayesian methods for assessing importance of
effects. Psychol Bull 119:149158
Sakai N (2004) The effect of visual stimuli on perception of flavor.
Poster presented at ISOT 2004
Santangelo V, Spence C (2008) Is the exogenous orienting of spatial
attention truly automatic? Evidence from unimodal and multi-
sensory studies. Conscious Cogn 17:9891015
Sarinopoulos I, Dixon GE, Short SJ, Davidson RJ, Nitschke JB (2006)
Brain mechanisms of expectation associated with insula and
amygdala response to aversive taste: implications for placebo.
Brain Behav Immun 20:120132
Scanlon BA (1985) Race differences in selection of cheese color.
Percept Mot Skills 61:314
Shankar MU, Levitan CA, Prescott J, Spence C (2009) The influence
of color and label information on flavor perception. Chemo-
sensory Perception 2:5358
Shankar MU, Levitan CA, Spence C (2010) Does higher level
knowledge mediate the interpretation of multisensory cues?
Conscious Cogn (in press)
Shiv B, Carmon Z, Ariely D (2005) Placebo effects of marketing actions:
consumers may get what they pay for. J Mark Res XLII:383393
Skrandies W, Reuther N (2008) Match and mismatch of taste, odor,
and color is reflected by electrical activity in the human brain. J
Psychophysiol 22:175 184
Small DM (2004) Crossmodal integrationinsights from the chemical
senses. Trends Neurosci 27:120123
Smith BC (ed) (2007) Questions of taste: the philosophy of wine.
Oxford University Press, Oxford
Spence C (2007) Audiovisual multisensory integration. Acoust Sci
Technol 28:6170
Spence C (2008) Multisensory perception. In: Blumenthal H (ed) The
big fat duck cook book. Bloomsbury, London, pp 484485
Stevenson RJ (2009) The psychology of flavour. Oxford University
Press, Oxford
Stevenson RJ, Oaten M (2008) The effect of appropriate and
inappropriate stimulus color on odor discrimination. Percept
Psychophys 70:640646
Stevenson RJ, Boakes RA, Wilson JP (2000) Resistance to extinction
of conditioned odor perceptions: evaluative conditioning is not
unique. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 26:423440
Stillman J (1993) Color influences flavor identification in fruit-
flavored beverages. J Food Sci 58:810812
Strugnell C (1997) Colour and its role in sweetness perception.
Appetite 28:85
Teerling A (1992) The colour of taste. Chem Senses 17:886
Tom G, Barnett T, Lew W, Selmants J (1987) Cueing the consumer:
the role of salient cues in consumer perception. J Consum Mark 4
(2):2327
Triplett T (1994) Consumers show little taste for clear beverages.
Marketing News 28(11):2, 11
Urbányi G (1982) Investigation into the interaction of different
properties in the course of sensory evaluation. I. The effect of
colour upon the evaluation of taste in fruit and vegetable
products. Acta Aliment 11:233243
Valentin D, Chrea C, Nguyen DH (2006) Tasteodour interactions in
sweet taste perception. In: Spillane W (ed) Optimising sweet
taste in foods. Woodhead Publishing Limited, Cambridge, pp
6684
Chem. Percept. (2010) 3:6884 83
Veldhuizen MG, Nachtigal DJ, Small DM (2009) Taste cortex
contributes to odor quality coding. Chem Senses 34:A8
Verhagen JV, Engelen L (2006) The neurocognitive bases of human
multimodal food perception: sensory integration. Neurosci Bio-
behav Rev 30:613650
Williams AA, Langren SP, Noble AC (1984a) Influence of appearance
on the assessment of aroma in Bordeaux wines by trained
assessors. J Inst Brew 90:250253
Williams AA, Langron SP, Timberlake CF, Bakker J (1984b) Effect of
colour on the assessment of ports. Int J Food Sci Technol
19:659671
Wilson T, Klaaren K (1992) Expectation whirls me round: the role of
affective expectations on affective experiences. In: Clear MS (ed)
Review of personality and social psychology: emotion and social
behavior. Sage, Newbury Park, pp 131
Yeomans M, Chambers L, Blumenthal H, Blake A (2008) The role of
expectancy in sensory and hedonic evaluation: the case of
smoked salmon ice-cr eam. Food Qual Prefer 19:565573
Zampini M, Sanabria D, Phillips N, Spence C (2007) The
multisensory perception of flavor: assessing the influence of
color cues on flavor discrimination responses. Food Qual Prefer
18:975984
Zampini M, Wantling E, Phillips N, Spence C (2008) Multisensory
flavor perception: assessing the influence of fruit acids and color
cues on the perception of fruit-flavored beverages. Food Qual
Prefer 19:335343
Zellner DA, Durlach P (2003) Effect of color on expected and
experienced refreshment, intensity, and liking of beverages. Am J
Psychol 116:633647
Zellner DA, Kautz MA (1990) Color affects perceived odor intensity.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 16:391397
Zellner DA, Whitten LA (1999) The effect of color intensity and
appropriateness on color-induced odor enhancement. Am J
Psychol 112:585604
Zellner DA, Bartoli AM, Eckard R (1991) Influence of color on odor
identification and liking ratings.
Does Food Color Influence Taste and Flavor Perception
in Humans?
Charles Spence & Carmel A. Levitan &
Maya U. Shankar & Massimiliano Zampini